Berliner Boersenzeitung - Good neighbors: Bonobo study offers clues into early human alliances

EUR -
AED 4.141462
AFN 77.74631
ALL 97.842641
AMD 433.594768
ANG 2.017971
AOA 1034.536964
ARS 1308.311269
AUD 1.751226
AWG 2.031018
AZN 1.918597
BAM 1.947622
BBD 2.277298
BDT 137.824956
BGN 1.955827
BHD 0.425106
BIF 3357.222512
BMD 1.127561
BND 1.452826
BOB 7.793552
BRL 6.415029
BSD 1.127904
BTN 96.252263
BWP 15.159883
BYN 3.6911
BYR 22100.185904
BZD 2.265568
CAD 1.559371
CDF 3230.460586
CHF 0.935351
CLF 0.027603
CLP 1059.253044
CNY 8.123284
CNH 8.107098
COP 4651.446423
CRC 573.313064
CUC 1.127561
CUP 29.880353
CVE 109.806855
CZK 24.946183
DJF 200.389933
DKK 7.459078
DOP 66.592747
DZD 149.278868
EGP 56.115644
ERN 16.913408
ETB 154.237447
FJD 2.551104
FKP 0.837713
GBP 0.837253
GEL 3.08949
GGP 0.837713
GHS 11.617631
GIP 0.837713
GMD 81.184436
GNF 9772.22756
GTQ 8.66186
GYD 236.326078
HKD 8.838761
HNL 29.377429
HRK 7.535257
HTG 147.466039
HUF 404.41053
IDR 18396.375156
ILS 3.961639
IMP 0.837713
INR 96.370325
IQD 1477.548363
IRR 47498.486638
ISK 144.203564
JEP 0.837713
JMD 179.675563
JOD 0.79939
JPY 163.711592
KES 145.737689
KGS 98.605487
KHR 4515.121619
KMF 489.925103
KPW 1014.690215
KRW 1549.40289
KWD 0.346342
KYD 0.939887
KZT 577.042419
LAK 24355.597704
LBP 101059.049919
LKR 337.803586
LRD 225.580815
LSL 20.193049
LTL 3.329393
LVL 0.68205
LYD 6.166109
MAD 10.441336
MDL 19.495831
MGA 5118.688809
MKD 61.526392
MMK 2367.70666
MNT 4031.309232
MOP 9.106984
MRU 44.639751
MUR 52.036718
MVR 17.432469
MWK 1955.757384
MXN 21.845809
MYR 4.779741
MZN 72.062677
NAD 20.193227
NGN 1789.855989
NIO 41.506822
NOK 11.484069
NPR 154.003819
NZD 1.890975
OMR 0.433554
PAB 1.127904
PEN 4.106666
PGK 4.697401
PHP 62.846273
PKR 319.052355
PLN 4.239374
PYG 9009.370461
QAR 4.111926
RON 5.066466
RSD 117.220917
RUB 88.459847
RWF 1594.262435
SAR 4.229634
SBD 9.415995
SCR 16.030808
SDG 677.092912
SEK 10.881303
SGD 1.454615
SHP 0.886086
SLE 25.618327
SLL 23644.380257
SOS 644.6162
SRD 41.899582
STD 23338.225924
SVC 9.868825
SYP 14660.232054
SZL 20.181974
THB 36.910135
TJS 11.250758
TMT 3.9521
TND 3.37341
TOP 2.640859
TRY 44.098978
TTD 7.656124
TWD 33.587769
TZS 3041.030997
UAH 46.874726
UGX 4108.856712
USD 1.127561
UYU 46.884385
UZS 14535.354476
VES 106.945196
VND 29336.869227
VUV 136.144521
WST 3.036891
XAF 653.225768
XAG 0.033815
XAU 0.000344
XCD 3.047289
XDR 0.811565
XOF 653.228653
XPF 119.331742
YER 274.955356
ZAR 20.221501
ZMK 10149.395789
ZMW 29.80491
ZWL 363.074023
  • RBGPF

    2.5600

    65.56

    +3.9%

  • RYCEF

    0.0800

    11.65

    +0.69%

  • CMSC

    -0.0400

    22.09

    -0.18%

  • BCC

    -2.8100

    86.73

    -3.24%

  • SCS

    -0.2100

    10.29

    -2.04%

  • BCE

    -0.3200

    21.16

    -1.51%

  • CMSD

    -0.0800

    22.05

    -0.36%

  • NGG

    -2.0300

    72.67

    -2.79%

  • RIO

    -0.8500

    59.95

    -1.42%

  • GSK

    -0.2600

    39.18

    -0.66%

  • RELX

    -0.6100

    55.05

    -1.11%

  • JRI

    -0.0800

    12.74

    -0.63%

  • BTI

    -0.1700

    45.09

    -0.38%

  • VOD

    -0.1100

    10.39

    -1.06%

  • BP

    -0.2100

    28.94

    -0.73%

  • AZN

    -0.5800

    70.38

    -0.82%

Good neighbors: Bonobo study offers clues into early human alliances
Good neighbors: Bonobo study offers clues into early human alliances / Photo: Martin SURBECK - Kokolopori Bonobo Research Project/AFP

Good neighbors: Bonobo study offers clues into early human alliances

Human society is founded on our ability to cooperate with others beyond our immediate family and social groups.

Text size:

And according to a study published Thursday in the journal Science, we're not alone: bonobos team up with outsiders too, in everything from grooming to food sharing, even forming alliances against sexual aggressors.

Lead author Liran Samuni of the German Primate Center in Gottingen told AFP that studying the primates offered a "window into our past," possibly signaling an evolutionary basis for how our own species began wider-scale collaborative endeavors.

Bonobos (Pan paniscus) are our closest living relatives, alongside chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes), and the two species are also very closely related to each other.

But while encounters between chimpanzee groups are inherently hostile and often result in lethal violence, interactions between bonobo parties hadn't been as well examined.

That's because bonobos, an endangered species, are notoriously difficult to study in their natural habitat -- and they live only in remote regions of the Democratic Republic of the Congo.

With research on chimps dominating the literature, some scientists assumed hostility against outsiders was innate to human nature -- something we had perhaps trained ourselves to get past by inventing new social norms, even as that trait lurked deep inside.

- Female coalitions against sexual aggression -

In the new paper, Samuni teamed up with Martin Surbeck, a professor at Harvard who founded the Kokolopori Bonobo Reserve, to carry out a long-term study over two years.

"The first thing they do... is try to run away from you," Surbeck told AFP, explaining it took a long time for the bonobos to overcome their inherent fears of humans and behave normally.

Days began at 4:00 am and involved researchers trekking through the dark forest until they reached bonobo nests, then waiting for sunrise so they could follow the apes throughout the day, aided by indigenous Mongandu trackers.

Samuni and Surbeck focused on two small bonobo groups of 11 and 20 adults respectively, and found to their surprise they spent 20 percent of their total time together -- feeding, resting, traveling and more.

"Every individual is different," said Samuni. "There are those that are more introverts, extroverts, there are those that are more pro-social than others."

The team found that cooperation between the groups was driven largely by a select few who were more helpful within their own group. These individuals tended to connect with similar "pro-social" bonobos from the other group, creating a system of mutual benefit, or "reciprocal altruism."

The positive interactions occurred despite a low level of genetic relatedness between the groups, and despite the fact that reciprocity -- such as paying back a gift of fruit -- often took place much later, in future encounters.

Intriguingly, females, both within and from different groups, were found to form coalitions -- sometimes to chase an individual from a feeding tree, at other times to prevent a coercive sexual advance from a male.

"We don't see sexual coercion in bonobos, which is a common phenomenon in chimpanzees," said Surbeck. "One aspect of that might be due to those female coalitions, that help the females to maintain reproductive autonomy."

- Are we more like chimps or bonobos? -

The authors suggest their research offers an "alternative scenario" to the idea human cooperation is against our nature, or that we broadened cooperation with outsiders by first merging our extended families.

But "this does not mean that reconstructions of ancestral hominin species should be based only on bonobos," Joan Silk, a scientist at Arizona State University wrote in a related commentary.

There are other ways in which chimpanzees seem closer to humans than bonobos -- for example they more often hunt animal prey and use tools. Male chimpanzees also form strong bonds with fellow males and support them in aggressive acts, while bonobo males form stronger ties to females.

Understanding the natural selection forces that created these differences "may help to elucidate how and why humans became such an unusual ape," she concluded.

(K.Lüdke--BBZ)